TED演讲是由TED从每年1000人的俱乐部变成了一个每天10万人流量的社区。为了继续扩大网站的影响力,TED还加入了社交网络的功能,以连接一切“有志改变世界的人”。从2006年起,TED演讲的视频被上传到网上。截至2010年4月,TED官方网站上收录的TED演讲视频已达650个,有逾五千万的网民观看了TED演讲的视频。 TED是以下三个英文单词的首字母大写:【T】technology技术;【E】entertainment娱乐;【D】design设计.它是美国的一家私有非盈利机构,该机构以它组织的TED大会著称。TED演讲的主旨是:Ideas worth spreading.
- 演讲提示
- 演讲文本
- 中文翻译
Michael Shermer 说,人类倾向相信奇怪的事 - 从外星人绑票案到探矿杖 - 这行为模式来自脑内的两组最基本、最古老的求生技巧。他解释它们是什么,以及它们如何为我们惹上麻烦。
Michael Shermer debunks myths, superstitions and urban legends, and explains why we believe them. Along with publishing Skeptic Magazine, he's author of Why People Believe Weird Things and The Mind of the Market.
Why you should listen to him:
As founder and publisher of Skeptic Magazine, Michael Shermer has exposed fallacies behind intelligent design, 9/11 conspiracies, the low-carb craze, alien sightings and other popular beliefs and paranoias. But it's not about debunking for debunking's sake. Shermer defends the notion that we can understand our world better only by matching good theory with good science. Thus, in order to explore a conspiracy theory that pre-planted explosives caused the World Trade Center towers to fall on 9/11, the magazine called on demolition experts.
Shermer's work offers cognitive context for our often misguided beliefs: In the absence of sound science, incomplete information can powerfully combine with the power of suggestion (helping us hear Satanic lyrics when "Stairway to Heaven" plays backwards, for example). In fact, a common thread that runs through beliefs of all sorts, he says, is our tendency to convince ourselves: We overvalue the shreds of evidence that support our preferred outcome, and ignore the facts we aren't looking for.
Michael Shermer: Why people believe weird things
So since I was here last in '06, we discovered that global climate change is turning out to be a pretty serious issue, so we covered that fairly extensively in Skeptic magazine. We investigate all kinds of scientific and quasi-scientific controversies, but it turns out we don't have to worry about any of this because the world's going to end in 2012.
Another update: You will recall I introduced you guys to the Quadro Tracker. It's like a water dowsing device. It's just a hollow piece of plastic with an antenna that swivels around. And you walk around, and it points to things. Like if you're looking for marijuana in students' lockers, it'll point right to somebody. Oh, sorry. (Laughter) This particular one that was given to me finds golf balls, especially if you're at a golf course and you check under enough bushes. Well, under the category of "What's the harm of silly stuff like this?" this device, the ADE 651, was sold to the Iraqi government for 40,000 dollars apiece. It's just like this one, completely worthless, in which it allegedly worked by "electrostatic magnetic ion attraction," which translates to "pseudoscientific baloney" -- would be the nice word -- in which you string together a bunch of words that sound good, but it does absolutely nothing. In this case, at trespass points, allowing people to go through because your little tracker device said they were okay, actually cost lives. So there is a danger to pseudoscience, in believing in this sort of thing.
So what I want to talk about today is belief. I want to believe, and you do too. And in fact, I think my thesis here is that belief is the natural state of things. It is the default option. We just believe. We believe all sorts of things. Belief is natural; disbelief, skepticism, science, is not natural. It's more difficult. It's uncomfortable to not believe things. So like Fox Mulder on "X-Files," who wants to believe in UFOs? Well, we all do, and the reason for that is because we have a belief engine in our brains. Essentially, we are pattern-seeking primates. We connect the dots: A is connected to B; B is connected to C. And sometimes A really is connected to B, and that's called association learning.
We find patterns, we make those connections, whether it's Pavlov's dog here associating the sound of the bell with the food, and then he salivates to the sound of the bell, or whether it's a Skinnerian rat, in which he's having an association between his behavior and a reward for it, and therefore he repeats the behavior. In fact, what Skinner discovered is that, if you put a pigeon in a box like this, and he has to press one of these two keys, and he tries to figure out what the pattern is, and you give him a little reward in the hopper box there -- if you just randomly assign rewards such that there is no pattern, they will figure out any kind of pattern. And whatever they were doing just before they got the reward, they repeat that particular pattern. Sometimes it was even spinning around twice counterclockwise, once clockwise and peck the key twice. And that's called superstition, and that, I'm afraid, we will always have with us.
I call this process "patternicity" -- that is, the tendency to find meaningful patterns in both meaningful and meaningless noise. When we do this process, we make two types of errors. A Type I error, or false positive, is believing a pattern is real when it's not. Our second type of error is a false negative. A Type II error is not believing a pattern is real when it is. So let's do a thought experiment. You are a hominid three million years ago walking on the plains of Africa. Your name is Lucy, okay? And you hear a rustle in the grass. Is it a dangerous predator, or is it just the wind? Your next decision could be the most important one of your life. Well, if you think that the rustle in the grass is a dangerous predator and it turns out it's just the wind, you've made an error in cognition, made a Type I error, false positive. But no harm. You just move away. You're more cautious. You're more vigilant. On the other hand, if you believe that the rustle in the grass is just the wind, and it turns out it's a dangerous predator, you're lunch. You've just won a Darwin award. You've been taken out of the gene pool.
Now the problem here is that patternicities will occur whenever the cost of making a Type I error is less than the cost of making a Type II error. This is the only equation in the talk by the way. We have a pattern detection problem that is assessing the difference between a Type I and a Type II error is highly problematic, especially in split-second, life-and-death situations. So the default position is just: Believe all patterns are real -- All rustles in the grass are dangerous predators and not just the wind. And so I think that we evolved ... there was a natural selection for the propensity for our belief engines, our pattern-seeking brain processes, to always find meaningful patterns and infuse them with these sort of predatory or intentional agencies that I'll come back to.
So for example, what do you see here? It's a horse head, that's right. It looks like a horse. It must be a horse. That's a pattern. And is it really a horse? Or is it more like a frog? See, our pattern detection device, which appears to be located in the anterior cingulate cortex -- it's our little detection device there -- can be easily fooled, and this is the problem. For example, what do you see here? Yes, of course, it's a cow. Once I prime the brain -- it's called cognitive priming -- once I prime the brain to see it, it pops back out again even without the pattern that I've imposed on it. And what do you see here? Some people see a Dalmatian dog. Yes, there it is. And there's the prime. So when I go back without the prime, your brain already has the model so you can see it again. What do you see here? Planet Saturn. Yes, that's good. How about here? Just shout out anything you see. That's a good audience, Chris. Because there's nothing in this. Well, allegedly there's nothing.
This is an experiment done by Jennifer Whitson at U.T. Austin on corporate environments and whether feelings of uncertainty and out of control makes people see illusory patterns. That is, almost everybody sees the planet Saturn. People that are put in a condition of feeling out of control are more likely to see something in this, which is allegedly patternless. In other words, the propensity to find these patterns goes up when there's a lack of control. For example, baseball players are notoriously superstitious when they're batting, but not so much when they're fielding. Because fielders are successful 90 to 95 percent of the time. The best batters fail seven out of 10 times. So their superstitions, their patternicities, are all associated with feelings of lack of control and so forth.
What do you see in this particular one here, in this field? Anybody see an object there? There actually is something here, but it's degraded. While you're thinking about that, this was an experiment done by Susan Blackmore, a psychologist in England, who showed subjects this degraded image and then ran a correlation between their scores on an ESP test: How much did they believe in the paranormal, supernatural, angels and so forth. And those who scored high on the ESP scale, tended to not only see more patterns in the degraded images but incorrect patterns. Here is what you show subjects. The fish is degraded 20 percent, 50 percent and then the one I showed you, 70 percent.
A similar experiment was done by another [Swiss] psychologist named Peter Brugger, who found significantly more meaningful patterns were perceived on the right hemisphere, via the left visual field, than the left hemisphere. So if you present subjects the images such that it's going to end up on the right hemisphere instead of the left, then they're more likely to see patterns than if you put it on the left hemisphere. Our right hemisphere appears to be where a lot of this patternicity occurs. So what we're trying to do is bore into the brain to see where all this happens.
Brugger and his colleague, Christine Mohr, gave subjects L-DOPA. L-DOPA's a drug, as you know, given for treating Parkinson's disease, which is related to a decrease in dopamine. L-DOPA increases dopamine. An increase of dopamine caused subjects to see more patterns than those that did not receive the dopamine. So dopamine appears to be the drug associated with patternicity. In fact, neuroleptic drugs that are used to eliminate psychotic behavior, things like paranoia, delusions and hallucinations, these are patternicities. They're incorrect patterns. They're false positives. They're Type I errors. And if you give them drugs that are dopamine antagonists, they go away. That is, you decrease the amount of dopamine, and their tendency to see patterns like that decreases. On the other hand, amphetamines like cocaine are dopamine agonists. They increase the amount of dopamine. So you're more likely to feel in a euphoric state, creativity, find more patterns.
In fact, I saw Robin Williams recently talk about how he thought he was much funnier when he was doing cocaine, when he had that issue, than now. So perhaps more dopamine is related to more creativity. Dopamine, I think, changes our signal-to-noise ratio. That is, how accurate we are in finding patterns. If it's too low, you're more likely to make too many Type II errors. You miss the real patterns. You don't want to be too skeptical. If you're too skeptical, you'll miss the really interesting good ideas. Just right, you're creative, and yet you don't fall for too much baloney. Too high and maybe you see patterns everywhere. Every time somebody looks at you, you think people are staring at you. You think people are talking about you. And if you go too far on that, that's just simply labeled as madness. It's a distinction perhaps we might make between two Nobel laureates, Richard Feynman and John Nash. One sees maybe just the right number of patterns to win a Nobel Prize. The other one also, but maybe too many patterns. And we then call that schizophrenia.
So the signal-to-noise ratio then presents us with a pattern-detection problem. And of course you all know exactly what this is, right? And what pattern do you see here? Again, I'm putting your anterior cingulate cortex to the test here, causing you conflicting pattern detections. You know, of course, this is Via Uno shoes. These are sandals. Pretty sexy feet, I must say. Maybe a little Photoshopped. And of course, the ambiguous figures that seem to flip-flop back and forth. It turns out what you're thinking about a lot influences what you tend to see. And you see the lamp here, I know. Because the lights on here. Of course, thanks to the environmentalist movement we're all sensitive to the plight of marine mammals. So what you see in this particular ambiguous figure is, of course, the dolphins, right? You see a dolphin here, and there's a dolphin, and there's a dolphin. That's a dolphin tail there, guys.
(Laughter)
If we can give you conflicting data, again, your ACC is going to be going into hyperdrive. If you look down here, it's fine. If you look up here, then you get conflicting data. And then we have to flip the image for you to see that it's a set up. The impossible crate illusion. It's easy to fool the brain in 2D. So you say, "Aw, come on Shermer, anybody can do that in a Psych 101 text with an illusion like that." Well here's the late, great Jerry Andrus' "impossible crate" illusion in 3D, in which Jerry is standing inside the impossible crate. And he was kind enough to post this and give us the reveal. Of course, camera angle is everything. The photographer is over there, and this board appears to overlap with this one, and this one with that one, and so on. But even when I take it away, the illusion is so powerful because of how are brains are wired to find those certain kinds of patterns.
This is a fairly new one that throws us off because of the conflicting patterns of comparing this angle with that angle. In fact, it's the exact same picture side by side. So what you're doing is comparing that angle instead of with this one, but with that one. And so your brain is fooled. Yet again, your pattern detection devices are fooled.
Faces are easy to see because we have an additional evolved facial recognition software in our temporal lobes. Here's some faces on the side of a rock. I'm actually not even sure if this is -- this might be Photoshopped. But anyway, the point is still made. Now which one of these looks odd to you? In a quick reaction, which one looks odd? The one on the left. Okay. So I'll rotate it so it'll be the one on the right. And you are correct. A fairly famous illusion -- it was first done with Margaret Thatcher. Now, they trade up the politicians every time. Well, why is this happening? Well, we know exactly where it happens, in the temporal lobe, right across, sort of above your ear there, in a little structure called the fusiform gyrus. And there's two types of cells that do this, that record facial features either globally, or specifically these large, rapid-firing cells, first look at the general face. So you recognize Obama immediately. And then you notice something quite a little bit odd about the eyes and the mouth. Especially when they're upside down, you're engaging that general facial recognition software there.
Now I said back in our little thought experiment, you're a hominid walking on the plains of Africa. Is it just the wind or a dangerous predator? What's the difference between those? Well, the wind is inanimate; the dangerous predator is an intentional agent. And I call this process agenticity. That is the tendency to infuse patterns with meaning, intention and agency, often invisible beings from the top down. This is an idea that we got from a fellow TEDster here, Dan Dennett, who talked about taking the intentional stance.
So it's a type of that expanded to explain, I think, a lot of different things: souls, spirits, ghosts, gods, demons, angels, aliens, intelligent designers, government conspiracists and all manner of invisible agents with power and intention, are believed to haunt our world and control our lives. I think it's the basis of animism and polytheism and monotheism. It's the belief that aliens are somehow more advanced than us, more moral than us, and the narratives always are that they're coming here to save us and rescue us from on high. The intelligent designer's always portrayed as this super intelligent, moral being that comes down to design life. Even the idea that government can rescue us -- that's no longer the wave of the future, but that is, I think, a type of agenticity: projecting somebody up there, big and powerful, will come rescue us.
And this is also, I think, the basis of conspiracy theories. There's somebody hiding behind there pulling the strings, whether it's the Illuminati or the Bilderbergers. But this is a pattern detection problem, isn't it? Some patterns are real and some are not. Was JFK assassinated by a conspiracy or by a lone assassin? Well, if you go there -- there's people there on any given day -- like when I went there, here -- showing me where the different shooters were. My favorite one was he was in the manhole. And he popped out at the last second, took that shot. But of course, Lincoln was assassinated by a conspiracy. So we can't just uniformly dismiss all patterns like that. Because, let's face it, some patterns are real. Some conspiracies really are true. Explains a lot, maybe.
And 9/11 has a conspiracy theory. It is a conspiracy. We did a whole issue on it. Nineteen members of Al Queda plotting to fly planes into buildings constitutes a conspiracy. But that's not what the "9/11 truthers" think. They think it was an inside job by the Bush administration. Well, that's a whole other lecture. You know how we know that 9/11 was not orchestrated by the Bush administration? Because it worked.
(Laughter)
(Applause)
So we are natural-born dualists. Our agenticity process comes from the fact that we can enjoy movies like these. Because we can imagine, in essence, continuing on. We know that if you stimulate the temporal lobe, you can produce a feeling of out-of-body experiences, near-death experiences, which you can do by just touching an electrode to the temporal lobe there. Or you can do it through loss of consciousness, by accelerating in a centrifuge. You get a hypoxia, or a lower oxygen. And the brain then senses that there's an out-of-body experience. You can use -- which I did, went out and did -- Michael Persinger's God Helmet, that bombards your temporal lobes with electromagnetic waves. And you get a sense of out-of-body experience.
So I'm going to end here with a short video clip that sort of brings all this together. It's just a minute and a half. It ties together all this into the power of expectation and the power of belief. Go ahead and roll it.
Narrator: This is the venue they chose for their fake auditions for an advert for lip balm.
Woman: We're hoping we can use part of this in a national commercial, right? And this is test on some lip balms that we have over here. And these are our models who are going to help us, Roger and Matt. And we have our own lip balm, and we have a leading brand. Would you have any problem kissing our models to test it?
Girl: No.
Woman: You wouldn't? (Girl: No.) Woman: You'd think that was fine.
Girl: That would be fine. (Woman: Okay.)
So this is a blind test. I'm going to ask you to go ahead and put a blindfold on. Kay, now can you see anything? (Girl: No.) Pull it so you can't even see down. (Girl: Okay.)
Woman: It's completely blind now, right?
Girl: Yes. (Woman: Okay.)
Now, what I'm going to be looking for in this test is how it protects your lips, the texture, right, and maybe if you can discern any flavor or not.
Girl: Okay. (Woman: Have you ever done a kissing test before?)
Girl: No.
Woman: Take a step here. Okay, now I'm going to ask you to pucker up. Pucker up big and lean in just a little bit, okay?
(Music)
(Laughter)
(Laughter)
Woman: Okay. And, Jennifer, how did that feel?
Jennifer: Good.
(Laughter)
Girl: Oh my God!
(Laughter)
Michael Shermer: Thank you very much. Thank you. Thanks.
自06年我上一次来后 我们发现了全球暖化 已变成颇严重的问题 於是我们在"怀疑"杂志 里了深入报导 我们研究了所有 科学及类科学的争论 不过最后发现我们根本不用担心这些 反正2012年世界末日就到了
另一件新消息是 应该还记得我曾为你们介绍过 Quadro 探测器 一如探水器 只是一个空心胶件上面装了会转的甚麽天线 你拿着走, 它就会指向甚麽. 若你要在学生衣物櫃里找大麻, 它会马上指向某人 哦, 对不起. (哄笑) 而别人给我的这只 是专门用来找高尔夫球的 尤其是当你在高球场里 而且钻够了树丛以后。 如果你认为这些东西"是有点傻但无害“ 这东西, ADE 651 以一只四万美金的价钱 卖给了伊拉克政府 跟这个没两样, 都是一文不值的废物, 被说成是靠甚麽 "静电 离子吸力"操作, 若译作 "伪科学胡绉八扯" 就好了-- 将一些动听的词儿凑成一串 却什么功能也没有 如果在禁区 你让人通过 就因你的这小探具说行 却有可能赔上性命。 所以说伪科学及相信这类东西 都有危险.
所以我今天要谈的是相信。 我想信, 你也是 事实上,我今天的主题是 相信是一种自然反应 是本能的一部分, 我们就爱信 我们什么都信 相信是自然的 不信, 怀疑, 科学, 都不自然 那比较困难 因为拒绝相信令人不适 就像X档案里的 Fox Mulder 谁想相信 UFOs? 嗯, 我们都想. 那是因为 我们脑里有个相信机制 事实上,我们是找寻模式的灵长类 我们找寻事物的关联: A和B; B和C 有时A真的和B有关 这叫做联想学习
我们找出模式,我们找寻事物的关联性 无论是巴甫洛夫的狗 对铃声和食物产生联想 于是一听到铃声就流口水 或是斯金纳的老鼠 对它的行为和奖赏 做出联想 于是不停重复一样的行为 事实上,斯金纳发现 如果你把鸽子放进这样一个盒子 它必须要按其中一个钮 他尝试找出模式 如果你从这里给它一点奖赏 就算你只是随意地给它奖赏 毫无计划的 鸽子仍然会找出一种模式 无论它们在拿到奖赏前做了什么动作 它们便会不同重复那动作 有时候是逆时针倒转两次 正转一次,然后琢按钮两次 这就是:迷信 很不幸地 我们也是如此
我称这个过程为“模式识别” 那是我们脑中将对有或没有意义的事物 都赋予模式意义的倾向 在这个过程中我们犯了两种错误 第一类是“假阳性” 相信一个假的模式 是真的 第二类是“假阴性” 第二类是不相信一个模式是真的 当它是真的时候。 让我们做一个思想实验 你是一个三百万年前的原始人 走在非洲大陆上 你的名字是露西 你听见草丛里有一些动静 是危险的捕食者 还是风声? 接下来你要做的决定将是你一生最重要的决定 如果你把草丛里的风声 当作危险的捕食者 你产生了一个错误认知 一个假阳性错误 但无所谓,你慢慢走开 你更小心、更谨慎 但如果你以为只是风声 结果却是危险的掠食者 你便成了它的午餐。 获得“达尔文奖” 从此以后从基因库中消失
问题是 只要类型一的代价 比类型二更轻微 “模式识别”就会一直存在 这是整篇演讲里唯一的方程式。 我们“模式识别”的问题 在于认清类型一和类型二的差别 这很令人困扰 尤其在这种刻不容发、生死交关的情况下 于是我们的内建反应 便是”相信所有模式“ “所有草丛的声响都是危险掠食者 而不是风声。” 经过一系列的演化 我们的信任倾向经过了一系列物竞天择的过程 我们找寻模式的大脑 总是在找寻有意义的模式 并且将这些模式与 一些令人恐惧的原型产生联想。
举例来说,你觉得这是什么? 这是个马头,没错 它看上去像是匹马,这一定是匹马。 这是个模式 这真的是匹马吗? 还是它比较像只青蛙? 我们的模式探测装置 在我们脑中的前扣带皮层 一个小小的探测装置 是很容易被骗的 举例来说,你觉得这是什么? 是的,是只母牛 一旦我启动了脑 - 称作认知启动 一旦我启动脑去看 它便会看见,寻找,就算我没去想模式 这又是什么? 有些人看到斑点狗 在这里,于是我们启动了脑 就算我们把指示拿掉 你的脑还是留着模式 你还是可以看到 这又是什么? 土星,没错 这个呢? 想到什么就喊出来 这群观众很优秀,Chris 因为图片裡什么也没有,据说没有
这是奥斯顿大学的 Jennifer Whitson 所做的一个实验 她想知道在企业环境中 不确定和失控的感觉 是否会让人看见一些虚幻的模式 就是说,虽然每个人都看得到土星 但如果你把人放在一个感觉失控的状况下 他就有可能在图片里看出什么 就算里面什么模式也没有 换句话说,越是失控的时候 想到这些模式的可能就更大 举例说,棒球球员在打击的时候 迷信是出了名的 但守备的时候却不然 因为守备的成功率 高达百分之90到95 就算最好的打击手,十次里也有七次是失败 于是它们的迷信,他们的模式识别 都和感觉失控有些 附带关系
你看见什么了吗? 有人看见任何物体吗? 其实是有的 只是模糊了 在你想着这件事的时候 这是英国心理学家 Susan Blackmore 做的一个实验 她让参与者看这个模糊的图像 然后再将结果和他们在 超感知测验的分数做比较 他们多相信超科学 超自然、天使种种 那些在超感知测验上拿高分的人 不但从图像上看到更多 模式 而且还是些不正确的模式 这是他们给参与者看的图像 这条鱼模糊了百分之20到50 刚刚你们看到的 是百分之70
另一个英国心理学家也做过相似的实验 他是 Peter Brugger 他发现右脑更能识别出有意义的模式 经过负责视觉的左脑 更甚左脑本身 如果你让参与者看这些影像 它们出现在右脑,而不是左脑 它们更容易识别模式 比你使用左脑时更有可能 我们的右脑似乎 更容易识别出模式 我们希望到脑子里看看 这到底都是在哪里发生的
Brgger 和他的研究伙伴 Christine Mohr 让这些参与者使用左旋多巴 左旋多巴是一种用来治疗帕金森氏病的药物 帕金森氏病和多巴胺降低有关 而左旋多巴会增加多巴胺 增加多巴胺让参与者 识别更多模式 比那些没有收到多巴胺的参与者更多。 多巴胺和模式识别 似乎有些关联 事实上 那些用来降低神经病症的神经安定药 偏执、妄想 和幻觉 其实都是模式识别的一种 他们是错误的模式。第一类的,假阳性 如果你给他们药物 阻断多巴胺受体 这些错误模式就会消失 意思是,如果你减低多巴胺 他们错认模式的几率 就会降低 从另一方面来说,像可卡因那样的苯异丙胺 那样的多巴胺激动剂 会提升多巴胺的分泌量 你更容易感觉飘飘然 增进创意,发现更多模式
事实上,我最近刚见过罗宾·威廉斯 我们谈论到他过去使用可卡因的时候更为有趣 当他还有药物问题的时候 或许更多多巴胺 代表更有创意 我认为多巴胺会改变 我们脑里的信噪比 决定我们辨识出的模式 是对或错 如果多巴胺太低,你有可能做出第二类的错误决定 忽略太多模式。你不想太多疑 如果你太多疑,你可能会错失一些很棒的点子 刚刚好,可以很有创意,也不会太容易受骗 多巴胺太高,变得过分敏感 每次有人看你,你就认为他们在瞪你 你觉得人们在谈论你 如果情况太严重 就会被贴上“疯狂”的标签 这就是费曼(物理学家) 和约翰·纳什(有妄想症的数学家) 两个诺贝尔得主的差别 一个感知的模式 刚好足够让她赢得诺贝尔 另外一个也是,只是有些太多了 就成了精神分裂症
信噪比让我们明白对模式太敏感或太不敏感都会出问题 你们都知道 这是什么吧 你在这里看到什么? 我现在在考验你的前状束 让你同时辨识出两种模式,产生冲突 这是一双Via Uno的鞋 拖鞋 这脚真的很性感,我必须说 虽然可能修片修的有点太过了 当然,还有一些暧昧不明的形象 持续改变着 似乎你在想的是什么 就会影响你 看到的是什么 我知道大家看到的都是灯 因为它是亮着的 当然,感谢那些环保运动 我们都知道海底哺乳动物的生存困境 所以大家在这里所看见的 一定是,海豚,对吧 这里有个海豚 这里有个海豚 这里还有个海豚 这里有个海豚尾巴,同志们
(笑声)
如果我们给你的影响在脑中造成衝突 就像按下了你脑中前状束的加速推动器 如果你看这里,没问题,看看上面,就产生了茅盾 于是我们把影像翻过来 你就能看出这是安排的 不可能的影像造成幻觉 用2D影像欺骗大脑很容易 你可以说“拜托,这很简单 大一的心理学课本都有” 让我们看看 Jerry Andrus' 创造出来的 3D幻觉方框 看上去 Jerry 像站在里面 站在这个“不可能的”方框里面 它很好心地加上了这个解释 让我们看出其中的玄妙 当然,摄影角度能改变很多事情,摄影师站在那里 于是这个板子看上去就像在这个板子上,这个压在这个上,之类的 但就算我告诉你是什么回事 这个幻觉已经在你大脑里留下了深刻的印象 大脑仍会找到这个熟悉的模式
这是一个比较新的例子 当我们比较这两个不同角度 大脑里产生的模式衝=冲突让我们迷惑 事实上,这是两张完全一样的照片 因为你比较这个角度 而不是那个角度 欺骗了你的大脑 于是,你的模式辨识程式又被骗了
我们很容易看到脸 因为大脑颞叶裡的面孔辨识软件 经过千百年的演化 变得特别先进 这是石头旁出现的一张脸 事实上我认为 - 这有可能是电脑做的 但没关系,重点是一样的 这两张照片哪张比较奇怪? 立即反应下,哪张比较奇怪? 左边那张。好的,让我们看看 是右边那张 你是对的 很有名的错觉 - 刚开始是撒切尔夫人 现在他们会定期更换政治人物 为什么会这样呢? 我们知道这错觉是在哪里组成的 在颞叶,右边,你的耳朵上面一点 一个叫做梭状回的小组织 有两种细胞负责 记得这些脸面特征 或是这些巨大的,快速的细胞 它们会先辨识脸的大概 所以你马上可以认出这是欧巴马 然后你开始注意到 眼睛和嘴巴那边似乎有些怪 尤其我们把它倒着放的时候 你便开始启动大脑里的面孔辨识软件
之前我提到一个小小的思考实验 想象你是个走在非洲大陆上的原人 想着到底是风还是掠食者? 这两者的差别在哪里? 风不是一个生命体 危险的掠食者却是有意识的作用者 我把这个过程称为 "造神“ (赋予作用) 尝试赋予这些模式 意义、意识、和作用 把它们想作是天上来的一些隐性的东西 这个想法是从 TED 的一位讲者 Dan Dennett 来的 它所谈到的"意向性立场"
我想这可以解释很多事情 灵魂、鬼魂、神怪、天使 外星人、聪明的造物者 政府的阴谋 全都是些隐形的作用者 用我们的有力的意向,去相信 影响我们的世界、控制我们的生活 我觉得这是万物有灵论 多神论和一神论的来源 一种相信外来灵体 比我们更先进、更有道德 而且故事每次都是 它们将会从天而降来拯救我们 聪明的造物主总是被描写成 这个绝顶聪明的道德模范 创造万物 甚至政府能够拯救我们的这种想法 已经不是很热门了 但我认为这仍然和”造神“有关 心理投射在我们至上会有什么东西 万能又伟大,要来拯救我们
我想这也是阴谋论的基础 某些事物正躲在某处,控制着我们 无论是个先觉者 还是标德堡秘密大会成员 但我们都有模式认知的问题,不是吗 某些模式是真的,某些不是 肯尼迪究竟是被杀手谋杀,还是背后有更大的阴谋? 如果你到那里去 - 一年四季都有人在那里 我在那里的时候,许多人告诉我狙击手的藏身处 我最喜欢的说法是躲下水道里 最后一秒从井盖跳出来,射杀了他 但当然,林肯便是被阴谋暗杀的 所以我们也不能完全忽略 所有的模式 因为,事实上,某些模式是真的 某些阴谋也是真的 这能解释很多事情
也有人用阴谋论来解释911 于是我们特别做了一整集的专题讨论它 19个盖达组织成员策划让飞机撞上大楼 这是阴谋 但“911真相组”不是这么想的 他们认为是小布希政府做的 我可以用一整个演讲来讲这个话题 但你知道我们为什么确定 那不是小布希政府做的吗? 因为它最后成功了
(笑声)
(掌声)
我们都是天生的二元论者 我们造神的心态 和我们享受这类电影的心态是一样的 说到底,都是因为我们的想象力 再来 我们知道如果你刺激颞叶 便可以创造灵魂出窍的体验 濒死体验 只要微微刺激颞叶 你也可以靠着催化离心机 便会失去意识 一旦组织缺氧 大脑便会产生 一种灵魂出窍的体验 你可以用这个,我试过 Michael Persinger的”神奇头盔“ 它用电波刺激你的颞叶 让你达到灵魂出窍的体验
我现在要用一个短片 作为总结 它有一分钟半 能证明我们预期和相信的力量有多么强大 让我们开始
旁白:我们在这里假装 要为一个护唇膏的广告片选角
旁白:我们希望可以选一些段落 在全国播出 我们要测试这些 护唇膏 这两位模特儿罗杰和迈特 会帮助我们 这是我们的护唇膏 另外还有一个著名品牌 你需要和这两位接吻 会有困难吗?
女孩:不会
旁白:不会吗?(女孩:不会。)旁白:你觉得没问题
女孩:没问题。(旁白:好的。)
这是个盲测试 我需要你 把眼睛盖上 你能看到任何东西吗?(女孩:不能) 把它往下拉一点,确定你不会看到下面(女孩:好的)
旁白:现在什么也看不到了对吗?
女孩:是的。(旁边:好的)
现在,我们所需要的是 护唇膏如何保护你的嘴唇 它的质感,对吗 看看你是否能察觉任何味道
女孩:好的(旁白:你有参与过任何接吻测试吗?)
女孩:没有
旁白:往这里走一步 好的,现在我要你把嘴嘟起来 嘟起来,然后靠近点,好的
(音乐)
(笑声)
(笑声)
好的 珍妮佛,你感觉如何
珍妮佛:很好
(笑声)
女孩:喔!老天!
(笑声)
Michael Shermer:非常谢谢各位,谢谢。谢谢你们。